Your browser doesn't support javascript.
Show: 20 | 50 | 100
Results 1 - 5 de 5
Filter
Add filters

Language
Document Type
Year range
1.
psyarxiv; 2021.
Preprint in English | PREPRINT-PSYARXIV | ID: ppzbmed-10.31234.osf.io.qresb

ABSTRACT

Background: In response to the Covid-19 pandemic, most countries implemented physical distancing measures. Many mental health experts warned that through increasing social isolation and anxiety, these measures could negatively affect psychosocial wellbeing. However, socially aligning with others by adhering to these measures may also be beneficial for wellbeing. Methods: We examined these two contrasting hypotheses using cross-national survey data (N=6675) collected fortnightly from participants in 115 countries over 3 months at the beginning of the pandemic. Participants reported their wellbeing, perceptions of how vulnerable they were to Covid-19 (i.e., high risk of infection) and how much they, and others in their social circle and country, were adhering to the distancing measures. Results: Linear mixed-effects models showed that being a woman, having lower educational attainment, living alone and perceived high vulnerability to Covid-19 were risk factors for poorer wellbeing. Being young (18-25) was associated with lower wellbeing, but longitudinal analyses showed that young people’s wellbeing improved over 3 months. In contrast to widespread views that physical distancing measures negatively affect wellbeing, results showed that following the guidelines was positively associated with wellbeing even for people in high-risk groups. Conclusions: These findings provide an important counterpart to the idea that pandemic containment measures such as physical distancing negatively impacted wellbeing unequivocally. Despite the overall burden of the pandemic on psychosocial wellbeing, social alignment with others can still contribute to positive wellbeing. The pandemic has manifested our propensity to adapt to challenges, particularly highlighting how social alignment can forge resilience.


Subject(s)
COVID-19 , Anxiety Disorders
2.
psyarxiv; 2021.
Preprint in English | PREPRINT-PSYARXIV | ID: ppzbmed-10.31234.osf.io.b8vfx

ABSTRACT

With restrictions on opportunities for face to face (FtF) interactions, Covid-19 lockdowns test the promises of digitally mediated communication (DMC) to foster social contact and wellbeing. In a multinational sample (n= 6436), we investigated how different modes of contact relate to wellbeing during a global pandemic. DMC was more popular than FtF and Covid-19 death rates played a bigger role in DMC use than state stringency measures. FtF contact was positively associated and messaging negatively associated with wellbeing. FtF was especially positive for people who did not perceive any loved ones in their household as vulnerable to the disease, yet did not vary with people’s perception of their own vulnerability. The results suggest that, in the face of the pandemic, men and women of all ages relied on DMC over FtF contact. Despite tangible costs to wellbeing, during the pandemic, people endeavoured to be physically distanced but not socially isolated.


Subject(s)
COVID-19
3.
psyarxiv; 2021.
Preprint in English | PREPRINT-PSYARXIV | ID: ppzbmed-10.31234.osf.io.edw47

ABSTRACT

How essential is trust in science to prevent the spread of COVID-19? Previous work shows that people who trust in science are more likely to comply with official guidelines, which suggests that higher levels of compliance could be achieved by improving trust in science. However, analysis of a global dataset (n=4341) suggests otherwise. Trust in science had a small, indirect effect on adherence to the rules. It affected adherence only insofar as it predicted people's approval of prevention measures such as social distancing. Trust in science also mediated the relationship between political ideology and approval of the measures (more conservative people trusted science less and in turn approved of the measures less). These effects varied across countries, and were especially different in the USA. Overall, these results mean that any increase in trust in science is unlikely to yield strong immediate improvements in following COVID-19 rules. Nonetheless, given its relationships with both ideology and individuals' attitudes to the measures, trust in science may be leveraged to yield longer-term and more sustained social benefits.


Subject(s)
COVID-19
4.
Jay Joseph Van Bavel; Aleksandra Cichocka; Valerio Capraro; Hallgeir Sjåstad; John Nezlek; Mark Alfano; Flavio Azevedo; Aleksandra Cislak; Patricia Lockwood; Robert Ross; Elena Agadullina; Matthew Apps; JOHN JAMIR BENZON ARUTA; Alexander Bor; Charles Crabtree; William Cunningham; Koustav De; Christian Elbaek; Waqas Ejaz; Andrej Findor; Biljana Gjoneska; Yusaku Horiuchi; Toan Luu Duc Huynh; Agustin Ibanez; Jacob Israelashvili; Katarzyna Jasko; Jaroslaw Kantorowicz; Elena Kantorowicz-Reznichenko; André Krouwel; Michael Laakasuo; Claus Lamm; Caroline Leygue; Mohammad Sabbir Mansoor; Lewend Mayiwar; Honorata Mazepus; Cillian McHugh; Panagiotis Mitkidis; Andreas Olsson; Tobias Otterbring; Anat Perry; Dominic Packer; Michael Bang Petersen; Arathy Puthillam; Tobias Rothmund; SHRUTI TEWARI; Manos Tsakiris; Hans Tung; Meltem Yucel; Edmunds Vanags; Madalina Vlasceanu; Benedict Guzman Antazo; Sergio Barbosa; Brock Bastian; Ennio Bilancini; Natalia Bogatyreva; Leonardo Boncinelli; Jonathan Booth; Sylvie Borau; Ondrej Buchel; Chrissie Ferreira Carvalho; Tatiana Celadin; Chiara Cerami; Luca Cian; Chiara Crespi; Jo Cutler; Sylvain Delouvée; Guillaume Dezecache; Roberto Di Paolo; Uwe Dulleck; Tom Etienne; Fahima Farkhari; Jonathan Albert Fugelsang; Theofilos Gkinopoulos; Kurt Gray; Siobhán Griffin; Bjarki Gronfeldt; June Gruber; Elizabeth Ann Harris; Matej Hruška; Ozan Isler; Simon Jangard; Frederik Juhl Jørgensen; Lina Koppel; Josh Leota; Eva Lermer; Neil Levy; Chiara Longoni; Asako Miura; Rafał Muda; Annalisa Myer; Kyle Nash; Jonas Nitschke; Yohsuke Ohtsubo; Victoria Oldemburgo de Mello; Yafeng Pan; Papp Zsófia; Philip Pärnamets; Mariola Paruzel-Czachura; Michael Mark Pitman; Joanna Pyrkosz-Pacyna; Steve Rathje; Ali Raza; Kasey Rhee; Gabriel Gaudencio do Rêgo; Claire Robertson; Octavio Salvador-Ginez; Waldir Sampaio; David Alan Savage; Julian Andrew Scheffer; Philipp Schönegger; Andy Scott; Ahmed Skali; Brent Strickland; Clara Alexandra Stafford; Anna Stefaniak; Anni Sternisko; Gustav Tinghög; Benno Torgler; Raffaele Tucciarelli; Nick D'Angelo Ungson; Mete Sefa Uysal; Jan-Willem van Prooijen; Dirk Van Rooy; Daniel Västfjäll; Joana Vieira; Alexander Walker; Erik Wetter; Robin Richard Willardt; Adrian Dominik Wojcik; Kaidi Wu; Yuki Yamada; Onurcan Yilmaz; Kumar Yogeeswaran; Rolf Antonius Zwaan; Paulo Boggio; Daryl Cameron; Michael Tyrala; Estrella Gualda; David Moreau; Jussi Palomäki; Matthias Hudecek.
psyarxiv; 2020.
Preprint in English | PREPRINT-PSYARXIV | ID: ppzbmed-10.31234.osf.io.ydt95

ABSTRACT

Changing collective behaviour and supporting non-pharmaceutical interventions is an important component in mitigating virus transmission during a pandemic. In a large international collaboration (Study 1, N = 49,968 across 67 countries), we investigated self-reported factors that associated with people reported adopting public health behaviours (e.g., spatial distancing and stricter hygiene) and endorsed public policy interventions (e.g., closing bars and restaurants) during the early stage of the pandemic (April-May 2020). Respondents who reported identifying more strongly with their nation consistently reported greater engagement in public health behaviours and support for public health policies. Results were similar for representative and non-representative national samples. Study 2 (N = 42 countries) conceptually replicated the central finding using aggregate indices of national identity (obtained using the World Values Survey) and a measure of actual behaviour change during the pandemic (obtained from Google mobility reports). Higher levels of national identification prior to the pandemic predicted lower mobility during the early stage of the pandemic (r = -.40). We discuss the potential implications of links between national identity, leadership, and public health for managing COVID-19 and future pandemics.


Subject(s)
COVID-19
5.
psyarxiv; 2020.
Preprint in English | PREPRINT-PSYARXIV | ID: ppzbmed-10.31234.osf.io.u74wc

ABSTRACT

Why do we adopt new rules, such as social distancing? Although human sciences research stresses the key role of social influence in behaviour change, most COVID-19 campaigns emphasise the disease’s medical threat. In a global dataset (n= 6675), we investigated how social influences predict people’s adherence to distancing rules during the pandemic. Bayesian regression analyses controlling for stringency of local measures showed that people distanced most when they thought their close social circle did. Such social influence mattered more than people thinking distancing was the right thing to do. People’s adherence also aligned with their fellow citizens’, but only if they felt deeply bonded with their country. Self-vulnerability to the disease predicted distancing more for people with larger social circles. Collective efficacy and collectivism also significantly predicted distancing. To achieve behavioural change during crises, policymakers must emphasise shared values and harness the social influence of close friends and family.


Subject(s)
COVID-19
SELECTION OF CITATIONS
SEARCH DETAIL